Skip to content

About a week ago I received my Middle Class Tax Refund from the state of California. Apparently we don't file our state taxes electronically, so instead of getting the money direct deposited to our bank account we got a debit card for $500.

I've never used a debit card other than my normal bank card, so I was curious how easy it would be. Here's my story.

  1. I called to activate it. With a normal card this takes about 30 seconds. With this one I first had to wait through a 60 second spiel about something or other, then had to press 1 to tell it I wanted to activate a MCTR card.
  2. The computer then asked for the last six digits of my Social Security number. This momentarily threw me and I entered the wrong digits. It eventually gave me a second try, and a little mental effort pulled up the correct answer. My card then got activated.
    .
  3. I surfed over to a website that showed me all the ATMs that would work for free. I went to the closest one but couldn't make it work. It kept showing me a screen asking where I wanted to withdraw money from: checking, savings, or credit card. None of them! I wanted to withdraw money from the debit card I had inserted.
  4. Confused, I went to the next place on the list. It was exactly the same. It was also broken.
  5. Then I went to a third place. By then I realized that all the ATM machines were Allpoint branded, so they all worked the same. No dice on the money withdrawal.
  6. Finally I went to my own bank, but the same thing happened.
  7. Feeling dejected, I went home. Eventually, Marian pulled up the 2-sided, small print instructions and showed me where it clearly said I should have picked checking in order to get my money. This does not strike me as intuitive at all.
  8. A couple of days later I paid for lunch with the debit card. It worked fine!
  9. So I went back to one of the ATM machines, and this time I chose the checking option and tried to withdraw $100. This got me a little further, but then it reported an error and ejected my card.
  10. The same thing happened at two other ATMs on two more days.
  11. WTF?

So that's where I am. I still don't know why I can't get money from it, though I suppose I'll eventually deplete it if I remember to use it to buy enough stuff. That said, I don't give the state of California high marks for all this nonsense. For me this is just a minor annoyance, but for someone who really needed the money it would be frustrating indeed.

I am bored. Kevin McCarthy just lost his 7th vote for Speaker, amassing his usual 201 votes. Matt Gaetz switched his vote from Byron Donalds to Donald Trump. Here are a few miscellaneous charts to keep you entertained while we continue this dreary process.

We're about to start the latest vote-a-thon for Speaker of the House, and it strikes me that the key question is:

Are there five Republicans who flatly won't vote for Kevin McCarthy no matter what? Deals don't matter, promises don't matter, ideology doesn't matter. They just won't do it.

Well, are there?

Imports and exports continued their downward trend in November:

Imports of consumer goods are down $23 billion since the March peak. Imports from China are down $11 billion. This is yet another example of the general decline of economic activity over the past six months or so.

Emma Fraser is really unhappy that Netflix has canceled 1899 after only a single season:

Nothing says ushering in the new year like attempting to break old habits. Netflix obviously didn’t get the resolutions memo, not even waiting until everyone had gone back to work before swiftly canning yet another ambitious series after one season. Yep, 1899 walks the same cancellation plank as shows like Archive 81 and The Midnight Club, with the news this week that, despite its popularity, Netflix is ending the series.

....So, why is this a big deal? Shows have long been dumped unceremoniously throughout the history of network TV....It is a business, after all, and only a handful of episodes have likely been shot in those cases; viewer investment isn’t as entrenched.

Flashforward (unintentional TV reference ahoy!) to the binge model and getting shit-canned after an entire season—complete with a tantalizing cliffhanger—is now the norm. Getting invested in a new series at all starts to feel rather pointless. It’s no wonder that, following the cancellation of 1899, people on Twitter are venting about how they’re so tired of getting hooked on a new Netflix series only for it to end after a season. The sentiment online is fueled by annoyance that this has happened yet again.

Two things. First, I'll bet that 1899's fans aren't so unhappy that they'll give up on Netflix—and that's probably all Netflix cares about.

Second, there's no telling why this happened. It might have been purely financial, but it also might have been a fight over creative control or something like that. Netflix might not be entirely to blame.

OK, three things. My advice is for Netflix to follow the Firefly model. It had a genuine following but wasn't profitable enough to keep going as a TV series. But it also had a bunch of unresolved story lines, so Joss Whedon shopped around the idea of a theatrical film to tie things up. Universal bit and the film was made. The same thing could be done with 1899.

Now, it's true that Whedon's movie didn't do well. But why not try again with 1899, except as a Netflix film? The great thing is that at the end of 1899, we discover that all the weird stuff happening is because the characters are living in a virtual reality created by a space ship in the year 2099. So instead of a weird, hard-to-describe puzzle box set on an ocean liner a hundred years ago, the sequel would be a nice, genre-friendly science fiction film that has a good chance of attracting a wider audience.

Why not?

The New York Times reports that the Fed is still worried about inflation:

Federal Reserve officials worried that inflation could remain uncomfortably fast, minutes from their December meeting showed, and some policymakers fretted that financial markets might incorrectly interpret their decision to raise interest rates more slowly as a sign that they were giving up the fight against America’s rapid price gains.

I get it. I'm not an economist, and it's easy to heckle from the cheap seats. If I were sitting on the Fed board I might feel more nervous about things.

But the Fed seems to have fallen completely into the rabbit hole of forward expectations. That is, they believe the Fed needs to credibly signal what it's going to do so that financial markets will respond "correctly."

There are some cases where this makes sense, mostly when you're dealing with a recession or a crash. If markets know that the Fed is committed to being a liquidity backstop, they will panic less and be less likely to produce a doom spiral.

But I just don't get the whole "forward expectations" school of thought when it comes to inflation. How are the markets going to respond? Will companies decide not to raise prices? Will workers decide not to ask for higher wages? I've never seen any credible evidence of this outside of hyperinflation episodes.

Rather, I continue to believe that inflation is fundamentally hydraulic. It goes up and down based on underlying movements of the economy, and that's it. These fundamentals might or might not include money supply, interest rates, tightness of labor markets, oil prices, endogenous variables, and so forth, all lagged by various amounts (oil hits inflation quickly, interest rates take a year or so). The Fed controls some of these, but (a) has only a modest impact and (b) has to be aware of lags.

In any case, I have no reason to think that expectations are on this list. Hell, Japan has been trying to raise inflation for years with no luck. And serious economists have taken to saying that nobody understands inflation. If that's the case, then why are so many people enthused about expectations, which strikes me as akin to clapping your hands or wearing a WIN button? It just doesn't work.

But I'm open to argument on this. What's the best shortish piece around on the impact of expectations on inflation?

For the past week or so Bob Somerby has been writing about whether doctors believe that Black patients are more tolerant of pain than white patients. In particular, he's been writing about a frequently cited study from 2015 which you can read here if you're so inclined.

Now, Bob is practically trolling me here. This kind of thing is right in my wheelhouse: read the study, explain the ins and outs, and discuss the results. But there's a problem: I've probably read this study half a dozen times over the past few years and I read it again last night. And I've never written about it because I've never been able to make sense of it.

This is weird because the study itself is the simplest possible kind: it's a survey of white medical students and residents—hereafter S&Rs. They were asked 15 questions about biological differences between Black people and white people, four of which were true and 11 of which were false. Here are the questions:

  1. On average, Blacks age more slowly than Whites.
  2. Black people’s nerve-endings are less sensitive than White people’s nerve-endings.
  3. Black people’s blood coagulates more quickly—because of that, Blacks have a lower rate of hemophilia than Whites.
  4. Whites, on average, have larger brains than Blacks.
  5. TRUE: Whites are less susceptible to heart disease like hypertension than Blacks.
  6. TRUE: Blacks are less likely to contract spinal cord diseases like multiple sclerosis.
  7. Whites have a better sense of hearing compared with Blacks.
  8. Black people’s skin has more collagen (i.e., it’s thicker) than White people’s skin.
  9. TRUE: Blacks, on average, have denser, stronger bones than Whites.
  10. Blacks have a more sensitive sense of smell than Whites; they can differentiate odors and detect faint smells better than Whites.
  11. Whites have more efficient respiratory systems than Blacks.
  12. Black couples are significantly more fertile than White couples.
  13. TRUE: Whites are less likely to have a stroke than Blacks.
  14. Blacks are better at detecting movement than Whites.
  15. Blacks have stronger immune systems than Whites and are less likely to contract colds.

The problem with the study is that after presenting the results of the survey it immediately dives into a long and messy bunch of weird measurements and unclear statistics. Its conclusion, based on the survey plus an additional set of questions, is that holding false beliefs doesn't much change assessment of pain in Black people and it doesn't substantially change recommendations of pain relief to Black and white patients.¹ The only noticeable effect is that S&Rs who hold a lot of false beliefs tend to have higher assessments of pain in white people.

That's a pretty weak result, especially given the deficiencies in the survey design. But for our purposes, let's keep things simple and just take a closer look at the survey responses. First, here are the basic results:

There are a few things to notice here:

  • Third years and residents generally do much better than first and second year med students on the false beliefs. Very few mark them as true.
  • Second years are more likely to mark something true whether it's true or not. They marked 31% of the statements true, compared to 23% for first years and 17% for third years and residents.
  • The respondents do worse on the true statements than on the false ones:
    Roughly speaking, about 10% of the respondents mark false statements as true. However, about half of all respondents mark true statements as false. Second year students do much better on the true statements but not so great on the false statements (or the pain statements).
  • Answers are given on a scale of 1-6. S&Rs are allowed to mark an answer as "possibly," "probably," or "definitely" true or false. With one exception, which I'll get to, virtually every single person who marked a false statement as true said it was only "possibly true." Among all the false statements, there were 229 marks of "possibly true" and only 9 marks of "probably true." There was not a single mark of "definitely true."

I said there was one exception, and this is it: I didn't count the marks from the question about the thickness of Black skin. This is the huge outlier, with 41% of first and second-years believing it and even 23% of thirds and residents believing it.

So what can we conclude? Not much, but I'd toss out a couple of things:

  • There does seem to be a problem with the belief that Black skin is thicker than white skin. This is worth addressing.²
  • Beliefs of white and nonwhite respondents are virtually identical. In particular the average score for nerve endings is 1.94 vs. 1.83 (nonwhite S&Rs are more likely to believe it) and 1.76 vs. 1.73 for skin thickness. Overall, the belief in false statements is 2.06 vs. 1.98, meaning that nonwhite S&Rs are slightly more likely to believe them than white S&Rs.
  • Belief in false statements is not a problem. The percentages are low and the responses are almost all tentative.
  • However, S&Rs are apparently so afraid of saying they believe in any Black/white differences that they do very poorly on the statements that are true.

Overall, this is a dog's breakfast of a study. The authors end up focusing on whether S&Rs who harbor more false beliefs also tend to rate pain lower in Black patients compared to better-informed S&Rs. It turns out they don't, but they do rate pain in white patients higher. However, the amount is smallish; it makes little difference in treatment; and the statistics presented seem cherry-picked and gnawed at a little too carefully. I'm not really sure I put much stock in the authors' conclusions.

I'd recommend that no one cite this study—and if you do, at least cite it correctly. The authors don't make this easy, but if you want to play with the raw data go here and click on "Study 2" and then on "Data - Raw and Cleaned." Be sure to first read the main report carefully, though. For example, you'll want to remove all nonwhite respondents since the study was solely of attitudes from white students and residents. And you'll want to understand the response scale. Etc.

Overall, though, I'd say you shouldn't bother. It's just a simple survey with weak results. The real question is why no one seems to have done much research on this question since 2015. The treatment of Black patients is an issue of high interest these days and you'd think there would be more about it.

¹The measured difference is 15% of a standard deviation. That's pretty small, the equivalent of less than a half inch in height in the human population.

²However, it's worth noting that this is an active area of research that has produced some contradictory results. See here, here, here, and here. That said, most pain isn't affected by skin thickness anyway. If you fracture a bone or have a heart attack, there's no reason to think white people will suffer worse pain than Black people.

He's still 20 votes short. A friend texts:

Are there smart insiders like Pelosi who know where this is all going?

As an experienced political pundit, I would say ¯\_(ツ)_/¯. Beats the hell out of me.

As near as I can tell the smart insiders are all hanging out on the House floor where they're getting drunk and trying not to laugh too loudly. Seems as good a plan as any.

Anyway, I'll put my money on Steve Scalise with p = 1%. What do I know?